Imágenes de páginas
PDF
EPUB

the same wall, it will then be in a proper position to bring parallel rays to a focus on the wall, where it will form an inverted picture of the window, and objects at a distance beyond the window.

If we now bring the flame of a lamp, for instance, to a distance of 60 inches from the lens, no distinctly defined image of the flame will appear upon the wall; but if, by any means, we can render the pencil parallel that diverges from the flame, the 12 inch lens will then converge it accurately to a focus upon the wall, where we will have an inverted image of the flame.

From the knowledge that we have now obtained, we know that a 60 inch lens placed in front of the 12 inch lens will render these rays parallel. All that we have to do then is to combine a 60 inch lens with the 12 inch lens the 60 inch lens to render the rays parallel that diverge from the flame, 60 inches distant, and the 12 inch lens to converge these rays to a focus, at the principal focal length of the lens. This is exactly what we do in supplying old people with convex spectacles. Their eyes are constructed to bring parallel rays to a focus, on the retina; but the rays from near objects are too divergent to be brought to a focus upon the retina without artificial aid; this deficiency is what we supply with suitable glasses.

Before leaving the consideration of optical lenses, there is one subject to which I wish to direct your attention; namely, the formation of an inverted image behind a convex lens.

Many of you are probably familiar with the fact, that when light is admitted into a darkened room, through a small orifice, there appears upon the opposite wall of the room, an inverted, dim, shadowy picture of buildings, trees, &c., in front of the aperture. This can also be seen, on a smaller scale, by holding a sheet of white paper a few inches from the key-hole of a darkened hall.

The cause of this is explained by Fig. 5.

[graphic][subsumed]

lamp that is a short Pencils of divergent

Let A B represent the position of a flame of a distance in front of an aperture of a darkened box. rays of light radiate from the apex of the flame in every direction: one of these pencils is represented in the figure to illuminate the end of the

box, and one of the rays escaping through the small orifice c; this ray passes in a straight line to the back of the box, and strikes the point a, which it illuminates.

Rays of light diverge from the lower part of the flame, also; one of these rays is shown to enter the aperture c, and to pass to the back of the box at b. In a similar way it might be illustrated that pencils of light radiate from every point in the flame A B, and that one ray from each point passes into the box and illuminates a portion of the back. In this way we have an illuminated spot at the back of the box, which is an exact counterpart of the flame in front of the box, but inverted, the apex of the flame pointing downwards. The reason that the picture is reversed is that, as rays of light (in the same medium) pass in straight lines, a ray from the top of the flame, after passing the aperture, must necessarily to the lower part of the back of the box; and a ray from the lower part of the flame must necessarily (in moving in a straight line) pass to the upper part of the back of the box. You will observe, also, that the size of the image depends upon its distance behind the aperture; if the image is as far behind the aperture, as the object is in front, the image will be of the same size as the object, if half the distance, half the size, as seen at fg.

pass

[blocks in formation]

If, in the above experiment, the aperture be enlarged, it will be found that the image at the back of the box will become much less distinct; the more the aperture is enlarged, the more indistinct will be the image. The reason of this indistinctness in the image is that, when the aperture is enlarged, a number of diverging rays from one point in the flame pass through the aperture, and each one repeats the image, so that the parts of the image overlap each other..

This is shown in Fig. 6. A B represents the flame of the lamp, and CEDF the image behind an aperture. The aperture is supposed to be just large enough to admit two divergent rays, each of these rays produces a separate image; thus, the point A is repeated twice at D and F, and the point B is repeated at C and E. The larger the aperture, the more light is admitted, but the more indistinct is the image.

If now, a convex lens be inserted in the enlarged aperture, these divergent rays that enter the aperture (from every point of the object) are converged to a focus; thus, in

[merged small][graphic][subsumed][subsumed]

Fig. 7. A C represents an object in front of a convex lens, and a c the inverted image behind the lens. Rays diverging from the point A and falling upon the lens L are brought to a focus at a; rays from B are similarly focussed at b, and so on. In a similar manner, diverging rays from every point in the object A C that enter the lens, are brought to a focus in the image between a and c. We will then have in the position of a c a distinct inverted image of the object A C. If this image is received upon a sheet of white paper, we can see it only upon its front surface; but if it is received upon thin oiled paper, or upon ground glass, we can see it from behind; and if, while viewing the image from behind, the ground glass be removed, we can still see the inverted image (or at least a portion) occupying the same position as the ground glass just occupied-being suspended, as it were, in the air, and forming what is called an ærial image. In order to see this aerial image under favourable circumstances, one eye only should be used, and should be in a line with the lens and the object, and should be at least ten inches behind the position of the inverted lens.

CHAPTER II.-OPTICS OF NORMAL EYE.

The human eye, from before backwards, is about one inch in diameter. Its transparent media are the cornea, aqueous humour, crystalline lens, and vitreous humour. This combination, with the convexity of the cornea, is equal to a convex lens having a focus of about one inch (more accurately 1 of an inch.

When a normal eye is directed to a distant object (i. e. in a state of rest), parallel rays of light are brought to a focus upon the retina, and a very minute inverted picture of the object is sharply defined upon that membrane. If the sclerotic coat be removed from the back of the eye of an ox, and the eye be placed in an aperture of a darkened room, with the cornea looking, for instance, towards the opposite side of the street, an

inverted image of the buildings, &c., in front of the aperture will be seen at the back of the eye.

The impression that objects make upon the retina, is conveyed through the optic nerve to the brain, but in what manner this communicates to the mind a knowledge of the appearance of objects, is more than we can tell. We can simply say with Potterfield, that "God has willed it so."

We are aware, however, that although the eye may be free from disease, and the connection between the retina and brain in every way perfect, if the optical mechanism of the eye be in any way defective so as to produce ill-defined images upon the retina,-vision will be indistinct, and that the distinctness or indistinctness of vision will be in exact proportion to the distinctness or indistinctness of the inverted picture. Hence the necessity of understanding the optics of the eye in order to comprehend the pathology and treatment of the numerous optical defects to which it is liable,

CASE 1.Let me here take an example. A few weeks ago a physician. of this city sent a patient for my advice, fearing that he was losing the sight of his left eye. Upon examination, I found that he had what we call "paralysis of accommodation" of that eye.

He could see distant objects with perfect distinctness, but near objects he was unable to define; he could not read large type unless the letters were very large, and several feet from the eye. The eye was, in fact, simply passive, like a convex lens, or a camera-obscura with the screen to receive the image immovably fixed at the principal focus of the lens, and could only bring parallel rays to a focus on the retina.

I found that by rendering the diverging rays parallel, by means of a convex lens, he could see near objects distinctly; by placing a six inch convex lens before that eye, he could read fine type at six inches, with a ten inch lens at ten inches, with an eighteen inch lens at eighteen inches, &c., &c. The six inch lens rendered the rays parallel that diverged from the letters six inches distant, and these parallel rays falling upon the eye were brought to a focus upon the retina. [A six inch lens does not increase the apparent size of letters one-half, whereas this patient could not see letters ten times the ordinary size at six inches, or any distance less than about two feet from the eye.] The ten inch lens rendered the rays parallel from objects ten inches distant, and the eighteen inch lens from objects eighteen inches distant.

The eye was unable to bring diverging rays to a focus upon the retina; in other words, it had lost the power of "accommodation." (We can tem

porarily paralyse the accommodation of the eye by applying a strong solution of Atropine.)

A normal eye differs from the glass lenses we have been describing in the fact that it can, not only focus parallel rays upon the retina, but also rays that diverge from objects as near as from four to six or eight inches from the eye. When parallel rays fall upon a one inch convex lens, they are brought to a focus one inch behind the lens, but if an object, for instance the flame of a lamp, be brought to within four inches of the lens, we know that the focus will fall further than one inch behind the lens. If we wish to receive the inverted image of the lamp upon a screen, the screen must be held one inch and a third behind the lens.

Now when an object is brought to within, say four inches of the eye, it has no power to move the retina backwards to receive the image that would be formed behind that membrane, but, what answers the same purpose, it has the property of so far increasing its refractive power, as to be able not only to render parallel these diverging rays, but also to focus them upon the retina. This increase in the power of the eye is equal to the addition of a four inch lens in front of an eye that has its "accommodation" paralysed, as a four inch lens renders rays parallel that diverge from objects four inches distant.

[graphic][ocr errors][subsumed]

Fig. 8 represents the section of the normal eye. When it is accommodated for distant objects parallel rays P P are focussed upon the retina at F, while diverging rays from O, would form a focus at fd. When, however, the eye is accommodated for the near object O these diverging rays are focussed upon the retina at F.

The manner in which this increase in the refractive power of the eye is effected is still a disputed point. Most physiologists, however, are now inclined to the theory that it is caused by an increase in the curvature, a thickening from before backwards, of the crystalline lens.*

The accommodation of the eye was at one time believed to be produced by the external muscles, but it is now ascertained that the accommodation can remain perfect with all the external muscles paralysed.

« AnteriorContinuar »